The icon indicates free access to the linked research on JSTOR.

Generation X has been called “the latchkey generation,” a group of people whose personal identity was in part shaped by the independence of being left alone after school. In fact, memories of spending time home alone still shapes the parenting of some GenXers, who vowed never to repeat their parents’ mistakes. But how bad was life as a latchkey kid, anyway?

JSTOR Daily Membership AdJSTOR Daily Membership Ad

Not so bad, wrote B. Bower in 1991, noting that latchkey kids “do about as well socially and emotionally as youngsters receiving adult supervision following classes.” Though children in multiple studies didn’t show gains compared to their supervised counterparts, writes Bower, better outcomes were linked to better home situations overall.

Deborah Belle attempted to qualify the term “unsupervised” in the late 1990s, noting that concepts of what was supervised and unsupervised varied by observer. In a four-year longitudinal study, Belle and colleagues looked at families with parents who were employed full-time and had at least one child in elementary school. Parents and children were interviewed separately about things like after-school routines and loneliness.

Belle found that some after-school arrangements “challenge the neat distinction between ‘supervision’ and ‘self-care’,” showing that either/or categories might not characterize unsupervised time well. Diversity was spotted in terms of after-school arrangements, caretakers, ability to invite friends over, and even where after-school alone-ness took place.

Children in the study could often call or visit parents, though some children preferred not to be called by parents while they were home alone. Belle writes that children “think about these arrangements and make sense of parental absence and their own lack of supervision in different ways.” Understanding their parents’ jobs and why it was important for them to work made absences understandable to some kids, and others found a sense of fun in their “home alone” hours. Belle concluded that while “self-care is a lonely experience for some children,” it was “a richly supported one” for others.

Looking back on these studies, the assessments of child mental health in relation to after-school lack of supervision is less surprising than the fact that such a lack of supervision was so common. Just a short time ago, kids’ lives often included significant time alone outside of the purview of adults. As Generation X continues to parent and their children come of age, it will be interesting to track the implications of constant child supervision and its absence from just a generation before.

Resources

JSTOR is a digital library for scholars, researchers, and students. JSTOR Daily readers can access the original research behind our articles for free on JSTOR.

Science News, Vol. 140, No. 4 (Jul. 27, 1991), p. 54
Society for Science & the Public Stable
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 43, No. 3, Invitational Issue: Child-Care Research in the 1990s: Child Care as Context and in Context (July 1997), pp. 478-496
Wayne State University Press